People with autism spectrum disorder sometimes acquire a new behavior or skill in a specific context, but then have trouble transferring the skill to a similar but different situation.

“Individuals with autism need to be taught in ways that support or promote generalization rather than in ways that reinforce over specificity,” says Nancy Minshew. (Credit: iStockphoto)

For example, children with autism can be taught what a dog is by showing them a picture of a dog and repeating the word “dog” over and over. But, when they are then taught what a cat is or even shown another type of dog, they don’t grasp the relevance of the earlier situation and have to learn this information from scratch.

Now, a new study shows that training individuals with ASD to acquire new information by repeating it actually harms their ability to apply that learned knowledge to other situations. The finding challenges the popular educational approaches designed for ASD individuals that focus on repetition and drills.

“There have been few systematic investigations into the fundamental mechanisms by which information is acquired by ASD individuals—and into the potential reasons for their restricted, atypical learning,” says Marlene Behrmann professor of cognitive neuroscience at Carnegie Mellon University. “This study begins to scratch the surface of the phenomenon.”

Using a computer screen, high-functioning ASD adults and control participants were trained to find the location of three diagonal bars surrounded by horizontal lines. Both groups were asked to identify the diagonal bars during eight daily practice sessions and their speed and accuracy were measured. The bars stayed in the same location for the first four days and were moved to a second location in the display for days five through eight.

“It was crucial to set up the experiment this way so that we could initially observe the learning in the ASD individuals in a simple, well-established task but then also document the difficulty in transferring the knowledge as the experiment progressed,” says Dov Sagi of the Weizmann Institute of Science.

For the first four days—with the diagonal bars in the first location—learning was equivalent for the ASD and control groups. However, once the location of the diagonal bars changed, there was a substantial difference. The control group smoothly transitioned to learning the new location and their performance continued to improve.

In contrast, the individuals with autism performed poorly when the target location was changed and they were not able to improve their performance, indicating that they received no benefit from initially learning the first location. Even more interesting, they were never able to learn the second location as well as the first, demonstrating an interference in learning that may reflect the consequences of extensive repetition.

“It’s like they showed ‘hyperspecificity’ of learning—their learning became fixed and inflexible—since learning the first location adversely influenced their ability to learn the second instance,” says Hila Harris, the study’s lead author from the Weizmann Institute.

Next, the researchers looked for ways to circumvent the hyperspecificity. With a new group of ASD adults and controls, they ran the exact same experiment, but this time they occasionally inserted “dummy” screens that did not contain any diagonal bars.

This time, when the location of the bars changed on the fifth day, the ASD group efficiently learned the new location.

“Our conclusion is that breaks in repetition allow the visual system some time to rest and allow autistic individuals to learn efficiently and to then generalize,” says David Heeger, professor of psychology and neural science at New York University. “Repeated stimulation leads to sensory adaptation which interferes with learning and makes learning specific to the adapted conditions. Without adaptation, learning is more efficient and can be generalized.”

The research team believes that the findings, published in the journal Nature Neuroscience, have important implications for educating individuals with autism.

“Individuals with autism need to be taught in ways that support or promote generalization rather than in ways that reinforce over specificity,” says Nancy Minshew, professor of psychiatry and neurology at the University of Pittsburgh. “For example, in the context of learning what a dog is, using a full range of examples of dogs—and even of animals, more generally—incorporates variability from the beginning and promotes learning a broad concept rather than a specific example.”

Researchers from the University of Haifa participated in the study.

The US-Israel Binational Science Foundation and the Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative funded the work.

Original study:  Perceptual learning in autism: over-specificity and possible remedies. Harris H, Israeli D, Minshew N, Bonneh Y, Heeger DJ, Behrmann M, Sagi D. Nature Neuroscience. 2015 Nov; 18(11):1574-6. DOI: 10.1038/nn.4129

 

View original article